Variability in Language Acquisition

So far we have described the patterns of language acquisition by using specific ages at which children reach different developmental milestones. We mentioned that children typically say their first words around their first birthday. We also said that their sentences become longer between 24 and 30 months of age. Typically developing children follow a consistent pattern of language learning. But it is important to remember that individual children progress at different speeds. There is a range of ages at which children reach any one of the milestones. Just like some children begin to walk earlier than others, some children begin to talk earlier than others.

You can see variability in children’s language acquisition in the graph. The graph shows vocabulary growth curves for typically developing children. The red line represents the average of the children. These children are in the 50th percentile. The yellow line represents children in the 10th percentile. Compared to their peers, children in the 10th percentile produce relatively few words. Their vocabulary is bigger than the vocabularies of only 10% of children their age. Their vocabulary is smaller than that of 90% of children their age. Now look at the grey line. It represents children in the 90th percentile. They have bigger vocabularies than 90% of children their age.

The graph shows that vocabulary sizes of typically developing children of the same age vary a lot. Some children reach the 200-word mark around 18 months of age. Other children in the 50th percentile know 200 words around 21 months of age. Children in the 10th percentile reach this point around 28 months of age. Note that all typically developing children reach this milestone, but at their own pace. The curves on the graph do not actually represent any one individual child. They describe general patterns in vocabulary learning.

Caregivers concerned about their child’s vocabulary growth should consult with the child’s pediatrician.

  • Back-and-forth or contingent interactions
    exchanges where a caregiver times her responses to a child’s behavior
    Canonical babbling
    producing the same consonant and vowel over and over, such as dadada
    Infant-directed speech
    a special tone and style of speech used to talk to young children. It’s also called parentese
    Joint attention
    shared attention between social partners to an object or event
    Overextension
    using a word to describe more object categories than it actually represents
    Underextension
    failing to extend a word to other objects in the same category
    Vocabulary spurt
    rapid growth in word learning